Laryngeal Motor Cortex and Control of Speech in Humans
Kristina Simonyan1,* and Barry Horwitz2
1Departments of Neurology and Otolaryngology, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York, NY
2Brain Imaging and Modeling Section, National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication
Disorders, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD
Abstract
Speech production is one of the most complex and rapid motor behaviors and involves a precise
coordination of over 100 laryngeal, orofacial and respiratory muscles. Yet, we lack a complete
understanding of laryngeal motor cortical control during production of speech and other voluntary
laryngeal behaviors. In recent years, a number of studies have confirmed the laryngeal motor
cortical representation in humans and provided some information about its interactions with other
cortical and subcortical regions that are principally involved in vocal motor control of speech
production. In this review, we discuss the organization of the peripheral and central laryngeal
control based on neuroimaging and electrical stimulation studies in humans and neuroanatomical
tracing studies in non-human primates. We hypothesize that the location of the laryngeal motor
cortex in the primary motor cortex and its direct connections with the brainstem laryngeal
motoneurons in humans, as oppose to its location in the premotor cortex with only indirect
connections to the laryngeal motoneurons in non-human primates, may represent one of the major
evolutionary developments in humans towards the ability to speak and vocalize voluntarily.
Keywords
motor control; voice; speech; human; non-human primate
Introduction
Voice is essential for human communication. Starting with the first cry at birth, the vocal
repertoire develops throughout childhood into unique human speech. The development of
the human ability to speak relies on the abilities to listen to speech, comprehend and process
the meaning of the heard words, and coordinate laryngeal, respiratory and orofacial muscles
to communicate speech sounds. Together with body expressions, we use speech and other
vocal gestures to define our needs and thoughts and to project our feelings and emotions.
Research on the mechanism of speech spans the centuries. Commonly, the topic of the brain
basis of speech is discussed in relation to speech perception within the auditory cortex and
speech production within the inferior frontal gyrus (i.e., Broca's area), while the involvement
of the laryngeal motor cortex (LMC) is rarely addressed. Nevertheless, the LMC is
imperative for the control of motor coordination of over 100 muscles for voluntary
production of voice, swallowing and breathing, all of which represent vital functions for our
existence and communication.
*Corresponding author: Kristina Simonyan, M.D., Ph.D. Department of Neurology Mount Sinai School of Medicine One Gustave L.
Levy Place Box 1137 New York, NY 10029 Phone: (212) 241-0656 Fax: (212) 289-4107 kristina.simonyan@mssm.edu.
Simonyan and Horwitz Page 2
In this review, we will first briefly introduce the anatomy and peripheral nervous control of
the larynx as an organ for voice production; we will then review the hierarchical
organization of voice control from brainstem to the LMC with a special focus on the role of
the LMC in the control of voluntary learned voice production and on its interactions with
other brain regions associated with speech control. We will present some hypotheses on the
LMC evolutionary developments in humans and conclude with a summary and future
directions.
Larynx and its peripheral nervous control
The larynx as a structure is phylogenetically much older than its role as a vocal organ. The
first vertebrate larynx appeared in the lungfish more than 400 million years ago; however,
the first vocalizations appeared much later with the evolution of anurans about 250 million
years ago.
From a phonatory point of view, voice is produced when the expiratory airflow from the
lungs sets the closed vocal folds of the larynx into vibration, converting aerodynamic power
generated by the thoracic and abdominal muscles (subglottal component) into the basic
sound wave (e.g., acoustic power). This sound wave is further filtered and amplified by oral
articulators, such as pharynx, tongue, palate, lips, and jaw (supraglottal component), and is
emitted from the mouth and nose as sound of voice (Fig. 1A,B).
Voice onset is required for production of vowels and voiced consonants (e.g., b, d), while
voice offset is necessary for production of voiceless consonants (e.g., p, t). During speech
production, voice onset is precisely timed, which allows linguistic distinctions between
voiced and voiceless consonants, such as /d/ versus /t/. Changes in the subglottal pressure
due to changes in lung volume, the elastic properties of the chest wall and the active
contraction of the intercostal and abdominal muscles lead to modulations of voice intensity,
whereas the resonance characteristics of the supraglottal region (e.g., oral and pharyngeal
cavities) influence the spectral properties of the sound.
Vocal fold movements are controlled by intrinsic and extrinsic laryngeal muscles. The
intrinsic laryngeal muscles are confined to the larynx and participate in vocal fold closure
(thyroarytenoid, TA, lateral cricoarytenoid, LCA, and interarytenoid muscles, IA), opening
(posterior cricoarytenoid muscle, PCA), and lengthening (cricothyroid muscle, CT) (Fig.
1C,D). The extrinsic muscles connect the larynx with surrounding structures, such as the
hyoid bone, sternum and pharynx, and raise or lower the larynx within the neck relative to
the spine to modulate vocal fold length, fundamental frequency, oro-pharyngeal resonance
frequencies and formant structure.
All laryngeal muscles, with exception of the IA muscle, receive bilateral motor and sensory
innervation from the superior laryngeal (SLN) and recurrent laryngeal (RLN) nerves
branching from the vagal nerve. While the internal branch of the SLN and the RLN provides
laryngeal sensory innervation, the external motor branch of the SLN innervates the CT
muscle; all other intrinsic laryngeal muscles receive their motor innervation from the RLN.
The extrinsic muscles are primarily innervated from the ansa cervicalis. The motoneurons of
the intrinsic laryngeal muscles are located in the nucleus ambiguus of the brainstem; the
motoneurons of extrinsic muscles are situated near the hypoglossal nucleus. Motor and
sensory nuclei of the subglottal (respiratory muscles) and supraglottal (oro-facial muscles)
components are located in the pons (trigeminal motor nucleus), brainstem (facial nucleus,
hypoglossal nucleus, and nucleus ambiguus), and ventral horn of the spinal cord (cervical,
thoracic and lumbar regions). Bilateral innervation of almost all laryngeal muscles and,
therefore, control of each half of the larynx by both left and right LMC is beneficial in
preventing loss of voluntary voice control in patients with unilateral LMC damage, while
Neuroscientist. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 14.
Simonyan and Horwitz Page 3
Two pathways controlling voice production
bilateral LMC lesions render patients unable to speak and sing. On the other hand, damage
to the branches of the vagal nerve results in the unilateral paralysis and paresis of the vocal
fold (Jurgens, 2002).
Central control of voice production is carried out by two parallel pathways: the limbic vocal
control pathway, which is responsible for the control of innate non-verbal and emotional
vocalizations, and the laryngeal motor cortical pathway, which regulates the fine motor
control of voluntary voice production, such as speech and song, as well as voluntary
production of innate vocalizations. These pathways are organized hierarchically, building
from the basic levels in the lower brainstem and spinal cord to the most complex levels in
the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and LMC, respectively (Fig. 2).
Voice control develops gradually throughout childhood for the control of speech production.
As we mentioned earlier, the first vocalization occurs at birth as a shriek of the newborn.
This and other types of non-verbal vocalizations in humans, such as an infant's cry and
laughter, represent innate vocal reactions whose acoustic structure is genetically
preprogrammed (Scheiner et al., 2004). This means that infants do not need to hear the
sounds of cry or laughter from others in order to produce them. These basic innate
vocalizations are controlled by the sensory and motor nuclei of the lower brainstem (e.g.,
ambigual, trigeminal, facial, hypoglossal, solitary tract nuclei) and spinal cord (thoracic and
lumbar ventral horn), which are responsible for the basic coordination of laryngeal,
respiratory and articulatory muscle activity (Fig. 2, subsystem I). The reticular formation of
the pons and lower brainstem is another important structure for this type of vocal control. It
plays a dual role in establishing connections between different phonatory nuclei and in
coordinating basic vocal motor activities. Single-unit recording studies in the squirrel
monkey have found that some vocalization-correlated neurons in the reticular formation are
active only during specific vocalizations, while other neurons fire during various types of
vocalizations or change their discharge rate based on the rhythmic frequency modulations of
produced vocalizations (Kirzinger and Jurgens, 1991; Luthe et al., 2000). Furthermore, the
reticular formation dorsal to the superior olive contains a vocalization pattern generator, the
neuronal activity of which co-occurs with the neuronal firing of the phonatory ambigual,
facial and motor trigeminal nuclei (Hage and Jurgens, 2006).
The involvement of the forebrain is not essential for production of innate vocalizations. It
has been reported that even anencephalic infants, who lack the entire forebrain but have an
intact brainstem, are still able to vocally react to painful stimuli (Monnier and Willi, 1953).
However, in older children, innate vocalizations come under voluntary control, sometimes
involving mimicking of vocal utterances. For example, cry can be produced in the absence
of pain or suppressed in the presence of pain. For this higher level of vocal control, that is,
voluntary initiation of innate vocalizations and control of their emotional status, the
brainstem phonatory nuclei and the vocal pattern generator require an input from the higher
brain regions, such as the periaqueductal gray (PAG) and ACC (Fig. 2, subsystem II). The
PAG receives direct projections from the ACC as well as from other cortical and subcortical
regions controlling limbic, sensory, motor, cognitive and arousal systems (Dujardin and
Jurgens, 2005). The PAG projects to the reticular formation of the lower brainstem, thus
representing an neuroanatomical and functional relay station within the ACC-PAG-
brainstem pathway. The PAG plays primarily a gating role in triggering a vocal response
and modulating its intensity, while the ACC is involved in voluntary control of voice
initiation and its emotional intonation. Destruction of the ACC results in a loss of voluntary
control of emotional intonations during speaking, while lesions in the PAG lead to mutism.
Interestingly, destruction of the ACC does not interfere with the production of vocalizations
elicited at the level of the PAG and brainstem reticular formation; however, destruction of
Neuroscientist. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 14.
Simonyan and Horwitz Page 4
Localization of the laryngeal motor cortex
the PAG abolishes vocalizations from the ACC but not from the brainstem reticular
formation (Jurgens, 2002). Thus, while the ACC and PAG are important in shaping
voluntary control of voice initiation and emotional modulation, the reticular formation
represents the basic executing level of this pathway.
The highest level within the hierarchy of voice controlling system is represented by the
LMC and its input and output structures (Fig. 2, subsystem III). The ability to learn new
vocalizations is very limited, perhaps impossible, in non-human primates and other
mammals (except whales and dolphins). Their vocalizations are exclusively emotional, with
each type of vocalization conveying only one meaning. In humans, in contrast, the
development of the ability to produce innumerable learned vocal utterances for speech and
song depends on the fine motor control by the LMC. While lesions in this region have
almost no effects on monkey vocalizations, they abolish speech production in human
patients (Jurgens, 2002). Such patients are occasionally able to initiate phonation, such as
grunts, wails and laughs, but do not succeed in voluntary modulations of pitch, intensity and
the harmonious quality of their vocalizations. The preservation of non-verbal vocalizations
in patients with bilateral damage to the LMC may be due to parallel organization of the
LMC and ACC-PAG pathways, one controlling voluntary voice production and the other
controlling the initiation of basic vocal reactions.
For proper coordination of learned vocal patterning and voice initiation, the LMC and ACC-
PAG pathways converge in at least two regions, as found in neuroanatomical studies in non-
human primates. One such region is the ACC itself, which has direct reciprocal connections
with the LMC (Simonyan and Jurgens, 2002, 2005a). The other region is the reticular
formation of the brainstem, which projects directly to the phonatory motoneurons (Hannig
and Jurgens, 2006). Thus, the vocal motor control system seems to be separated into two
parallel pathways for learned and innate vocalizations, coordination and interactions of
which are indispensible for proper voice control.
As we stated earlier, the role of the LMC is essential in the control of voluntary laryngeal
behaviors, both learned, such as speech and song, and innate, such as production on demand
of laughter, coughing, breathing, etc. The ability to control various laryngeal behaviors
voluntarily is most prominent in humans, while other species, including non-human
primates, have limited ability to produce their vocalizations voluntarily.
First observations of the larynx representation within the motor cortex were made in the
1930s. Shortly after the report by Oscar Foester that bilateral vocal fold movements in
humans can be produced with electrical stimulation of the motor strip of one hemisphere
(Foester, 1936), Wilder Penfield and colleagues described the representation of vocalization
in the inferior portion of the motor cortex above the jaw and below the lip muscles
representations (Penfield and Bordley, 1937) (Fig. 3A). Nearly the same somatotopy was
observed in the monkey brain with the representation of the larynx region just above the
Sylvian fissure (Woolsey et al., 1952) (Fig. 3B). On the other hand, electrical stimulation
studies failed to identify the larynx representation in the motor cortex of lower mammals,
such as the dog and cat (Milojevic and Hast, 1964).
Although the implications of the LMC in the control of voice production is significantly
different between humans and non-human primates, much of our understanding about the
organization of the LMC comes from research in non-human primates, primarily in the
rhesus and squirrel monkeys. The LMC in these species is situated between the inferior
branch of the arcuate sulcus anteriorly and the subcentral dimple posteriorly (Hast et al.,
1974) (Fig. 3D). This region contains small populations of neurons that are selectively active
Neuroscientist. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 14.
Simonyan and Horwitz Page 5
Laryngeal motor cortical networks
during either conditioned or spontaneous vocalizations (Coude et al., 2009). Among these,
the majority of neurons specific for conditioned vocalization discharge before voice onset,
while a smaller number of neurons are time-locked with voice onset.
The larynx area is surrounded by the representations of the tongue, lip and masticatory
muscles and is considered to be part of the primary motor cortex, although
cytoarchitectonically it corresponds to premotor cortex, area 6, in non-human primates
(Jurgens, 1974; Simonyan and Jurgens, 2002). Electrical stimulation of this region with
simultaneous laryngeal electromyographic (EMG) recordings in the rhesus monkey revealed
that the intrinsic and extrinsic laryngeal muscles have separate representations within the
LMC, i.e. topographical organization (Hast et al., 1974) (Fig. 3D).
More than half a century later after Foester's and Penfield's seminal observations, studies of
the LMC in humans have been recently revived with the advent of non-invasive
neuroimaging techniques. Recent investigations in humans have shown that the LMC is
located more dorsally from the Sylvian fissure (Khedr and Aref, 2002; Rodel et al., 2004)
than originally proposed (Penfield and Bordley, 1937). Similar to non-human primates, the
human LMC is organized topographically, although, compared to the monkey's brain, the
representation of the laryngeal muscles is reversed, with the CT muscle located more
medially than the TA muscle (Rodel et al., 2004). However, in contrast to the monkey,
recent studies have substantially revised the location of the LMC in humans. From the
earlier works of Penfield and colleagues, it is notable that the motor homunculus identifies a
representation of a behavior (vocalization) rather than an organ (larynx) in the inferior
portion of the motor cortex (Fig. 3A). As behavior, vocalization requires not only laryngeal
muscle activity but also orchestrated activity of the respiratory and orofacial movements.
Hence, it is not surprising that, on the map of the motor homunculus, vocalization as
behavior, in contrast to the single organ representation, encompasses quite a large region
within the motor cortex, overlapping with the lip, jaw and tongue representations, without,
however, a specific reference to the larynx. Thus, the location of the human LMC remained
largely unknown until recently. Neuroimaging studies using different vocal tasks were
finally able to separate and localize the laryngeal region within the primary motor cortex
(Brown et al., 2008; Loucks et al., 2007; Rodel et al., 2004; Simonyan et al., 2009; Wilson et
al., 2004), predominantly in the area 4p (Fig. 3C).
The differences in the cytoarchitectonic location of the LMC between humans and non-
human primates deserve special attention. The representation of the LMC in the primary
motor cortex (area 4) in humans, as oppose to its location in the premotor cortex (area 6) in
non-human primates, may represent one of the major evolutionary developments in humans
towards the ability to speak and vocalize voluntarily. The LMC representation in area 4 of
the motor cortex may have enabled the establishment of a unique direct connection between
the LMC and laryngeal motoneurons of the brainstem for faster neuronal transmission and
direct control over the coordinated activity of complex laryngeal, orofacial and respiratory
movements for speech production. In monkeys, this connection is indirect and hence the
control of the brainstem laryngeal motoneurons is limited. We hypothesize that3 this
neuroanatomical difference may underlie the very limited ability of non-human primates to
learn and control their vocalizations voluntarily.
Despite the differences in the representation of the larynx area in the motor cortex and in its
anatomical and functional distinctions between humans and non-human primates, the latter
species still remain a valuable model for studying the neuroanatomical projections of the
LMC using invasive tract tracing techniques. Such investigations in non-human primates
can be considered complimentary to diffusion tensor tractography (DTT) studies in humans
Neuroscientist. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 14.
Simonyan and Horwitz Page 6
for categorization of directionality of the LMC connections (efferent vs. afferent), which is
otherwise impossible to define using only the DTT approach. Hence, in the following, we
will review the LMC networks based on studies using both DTT in humans (Simonyan et
al., 2009) and neuroanatomical tract tracing in the rhesus and squirrel monkeys (Jurgens,
1976; Simonyan and Jurgens, 2002, 2003, 2005a, b). We will further review the interaction
of the LMC with the main cortical and subcortical regions indispensible for voluntary voice
production based on neuroimaging studies of voice and speech in humans.
A series of recent investigations have shown that both humans and non-human primates
share a common network of extensive cortical and subcortical connections with the LMC
(Fig. 4). Nearly all cortical connections of the LMC are reciprocal, that is the LMC both
receives and sends information to these regions. The regions reciprocally connected with the
LMC are the surrounding ventral and dorsal premotor, primary motor and somatosensory
cortices at their orofacial and trunkal representations, inferior frontal gyrus (IFG),
ventrolateral and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, insula, cingulate cortex (CC), supplementary
motor area (SMA), angular (AG), supramarginal (SMG), middle (MTG) and superior
temporal (STG) gyri, claustrum, ventral and mediodorsal thalamus, medial parabrachial
nucleus, deep mesencephalic nucleus and locus coeruleus. The only cortical region
projecting to the LMC without receiving an input from it is the orbital cortex, which is
assumed to have only a very indirect involvement in vocal control (Price, 1996), as well as
subcortically the ventral tegmental area, substantia nigra and raphe nucleus. On the other
hand, the cortical regions that receive projections from the LMC but do not send connections
back are the inferior parietal cortex, posterior parietal operculum and cortex within the
intraparietal sulcus, as well as subcortically the putamen, caudate nucleus, globus pallidus
and brainstem nuclei (i.e., reticular formation and spinal trigeminal, solitary tract and facial
nuclei).
Functionally, the LMC is connected with most of these structures to fulfill its main task - the
voluntary control of voice production. Interconnections of the LMC with the surrounding
somatosensory cortex and inferior parietal cortex are important for integration of
proprioceptive and tactile feedback from the orofacial, respiratory and laryngeal regions
during voice production. Neuroimaging studies of overt and covert voice and speech
production have consistently reported activation of the primary sensorimotor cortex (e.g.,
(Bohland and Guenther, 2006; Horwitz et al., 2003; Loucks et al., 2007; Riecker et al.,
2000). Activation in this region is organized somatotopically with the orofacial and
laryngeal activation occupying the ventral portion of the sensorimotor cortex and the
respiratory activation represented as two discrete loci in the dorsal region of the trunk
representation and in the ventral region overlapping with the laryngeal and orofacial
activation (Brown et al., 2009; Loucks et al., 2007; Simonyan et al., 2007). Interestingly,
only voluntary exhalation, which is required for voice production, but not inhalation elicits
activation of both dorsal and ventral sensorimotor cortex (Ramsay et al., 1993). The
orchestration of simultaneous laryngeal, orofacial and respiratory muscle activity at the
cortical sensorimotor level becomes apparent when one of its components is voluntarily
modulated or altered. A recent study of functional LMC networks during syllable production
showed that when subjects were asked to produce voice with minimal orofacial movements
and at the similar breathing depth, that is the modulation of two out of three components of
voice production system, the LMC displayed reduced functional connectivity with both
orofacial and respiratory sensorimotor cortices to maintain specific level of task production
(Simonyan et al., 2009). The influences of the somatosensory cortex on the motor cortex
become even more apparent when the auditory feedback, another online monitoring system
of correct voice and speech production, is altered and the encoding of auditory input is
challenged, for example, in the presence of very loud noise (Guenther, 2006; Peschke et al.,
2009). These observations suggest that the ventral sensorimotor region contains not only a
Neuroscientist. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 14.
Simonyan and Horwitz Page 7
simple ensemble of motor and sensory components of the voice controlling system (e.g.,
laryngeal, orofacial and respiratory), but it also acts as a higher-level centralized region,
coordinating exhalatory airflow necessary for setting the vocal folds into vibration and
modulating the orofacial articulators for voluntary production of different types of vocal
behaviors.
With respect to the inferior parietal cortex, including the SMG and AG, this region
represents one of a few higher-order sensorimotor centers for coordination of both speech
production and comprehension. The inferior parietal cortex is known to be involved in
complex phonological and semantic processing and monitoring of the verbal response to
suppress phonemic errors (e.g., (Fiebach et al., 2007; Hocking et al., 2009; Zheng et al.,
2010)).
The LMC requires an input from the IFG for motor planning of voice and speech
production. The IFG is another brain region where the control of speech production and
comprehension converge. Its pars orbitalis is involved in the retrieval of semantic
information (de Zubicaray and McMahon, 2009; Tyler et al., 2010) and its pars opercularis
is responsible for hierarchical sequencing of linguistic and non-linguistic sequences
(Tettamanti et al., 2009; Willems et al., 2009) and articulatory preparation to speech
production (Papoutsi et al., 2009; Zheng et al., 2010). Because the IFG is prominent in the
processing of information necessary for long-term preparation of learned oro-motor
sequences, it is not surprising that the IFG is usually active during production of long
sequences of syllables and words (e.g., (Horwitz et al., 2003; Ozdemir et al., 2006; Wise et
al., 1999) and only rarely during production of single syllables (Brown et al., 2008; Ghosh et
al., 2008; Loucks et al., 2007). Despite the fact that the IFG activation is not always
observed in studies of differential complexity of voice and speech production, the strong
functional and structural connectivity between the IFG and LMC seems to be responsible for
motor preparation and processing of all components of speech production (e.g., laryngeal,
orofacial, or respiratory) (Greenlee et al., 2004; Simonyan et al., 2009).
The reciprocal connections of the LMC with the SMA are needed for preparation for vocal
motor command execution. Accordingly, it has been shown that the offset of the late vocal-
related cortical potentials in the SMA precedes those in the LMC pointing to the
involvement of the LMC only after the motor preparatory phase in the SMA (Galgano and
Froud, 2008). The SMA is active during various voluntary laryngeal tasks (e.g., (Ghosh et
al., 2008; Loucks et al., 2007; Ozdemir et al., 2006; Simonyan et al., 2007). More
specifically, it has been shown that activation in the anterior pre-SMA was related to
effortful word selection, activation in the posterior pre-SMA was related to the control of
syllable sequencing, and activation in the SMA proper was related to overt articulation
(Alario et al., 2006). Recently, the SMA has also been identified as a motor component of
the speech monitoring network (van de Ven et al., 2009). Furthermore, electrical stimulation
of this region has been reported to elicit vocalizations in humans (Penfield and Welch, 1951)
but not in monkeys (Jurgens, 2002). Similarly, bilateral lesions in the SMA have no effect
on monkey vocalizations. In contrast, in humans, such lesions severely reduce motivation to
employ propositional speech, whereas nonpropositional (automatic) speech remains almost
intact (so-called transcortical motor aphasia). Functional connections between the LMC and
SMA are left-lateralized and are particularly stronger with the pre-SMA during learned
voice production compared to innate laryngeal behavior, such as breathing (Simonyan et al.,
2009). It, thus, appears that the connectivity between the LMC and the pre-SMA, a region
responsible for processing of higher order motor plans for subsequently ordered movement
execution (Matsuzaka et al., 1992; Shima and Tanji, 1998; Tanji and Shima, 1996), plays a
central role in sequencing and initiation of complex learned vocal movements during speech
production.
Neuroscientist. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 14.
Simonyan and Horwitz Page 8
It is important to note that the LMC has a reciprocal structural connection with the ACC but
not with the PAG as identified based on neuroanatomical tract tracing studies in non-human
primates. The lack of connections between the LMC and PAG fits well with the separation
between the two descending pathways controlling voice production, limbic and motor
cortical. Similarly, in the monkey, the PAG blockade has no effect on vocal fold movements
elicited from the LMC as oppose to the drastic effects on vocalization elicited by limbic
structures (Jurgens, 2002). The LMC, thus, seems to control the voluntary voice production
via a pathway by-passing the PAG, although it establishes connections with the ACC for
shaping emotional intonations during speech production. As noted earlier, the connection
between the LMC and ACC serves as one of the two links at which two pathways
controlling innate emotional and learned vocalizations converge.
Another link between the limbic and motor cortical vocal pathways is present at the level of
the brainstem reticular formation, specifically in its dorsal and parvocellular reticular nuclei,
which are further directly connected with laryngeal motoneurons in the nucleus ambiguus,
the articulatory motoneurons in the trigeminal motor, facial and hypoglossal nuclei, and the
expiratory motoneurons in the thoracic and upper lumbar spinal cord (Bernard et al., 1990;
Thoms and Jurgens, 1987; VanderHorst et al., 2001). Combined electrical stimulation and
lesioning studies have shown that bilateral lesions in this region block vocalizations elicited
from the PAG and vocal fold movements elicited from the LMC (Jurgens and Ehrenreich,
2007; Shiba et al., 1997). Because both limbic and motor cortical pathways come together in
the reticular formation of the brainstem, it has been suggested that this region is involved in
vocal motor coordination of both innate and learned voice production (Jurgens and
Ehrenreich, 2007). Its functional properties are more important in vocal motor control in
non-human primates than in humans due to the lack of direct projections from the LMC to
the nucleus ambiguus in the former species and, therefore, their reduced ability to directly
modulate activity of brainstem laryngeal motoneurons (Jurgens, 1976; Simonyan and
Jurgens, 2003). In humans, in contrast, direct connections do exist between the LMC and the
nucleus ambiguus (Iwatsubo et al., 1990; Kuypers, 1958). This direct LMC-ambigual
connection together with the LMC representation in the primary motor cortex seem to be
very recent acquisitions in hominid evolution and may be regarded crucial as prerequisites
for speech control.
In addition to direct LMC-brainstem laryngeal motoneurons connections, the LMC
establishes a widespread network of subcortical connections, most of which project further
down to the brainstem phonatory motoneurons. The putamen receives the strongest of all
telencephalic subcortical projections from the LMC, thus representing the main basal
ganglia output structure of the LMC. Putaminal lesions cause dysarthria and dysphonia in
humans but have no effect on monkey's vocalizations (Jurgens, 2002), suggesting the
involvement of the putamen only in learned voluntary voice and speech production but not
in production of innate vocalizations. Motor cortical connections to the putamen are
somatotopically organized with the leg area projecting to the rostrodorsal putamen, the arm
and trunk occupying its central part, the face area projections to the caudoventral putamen,
and the larynx area connecting with the ventral putamen over a large anterio-posterior extent
(Kunzle, 1975; Simonyan and Jurgens, 2003). It appears that there is an overlap of
projections from the face and larynx areas in the posterior (postcommissural) putamen (i.e.,
part of the sensorimotor functional striatal loop), whereas the putamen rostral to the anterior
commissure (i.e., part of the associative and limbic striatal loops) receives input only from
the LMC. Direct connections of the LMC with all striatal functional subdivisions, including
the sensorimotor, associative and limbic loops, may represent important factors in the
integrative control of different aspects of speech production, ranging from motor control to
motivation and cognitive processing of speech.
Neuroscientist. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 14.
Simonyan and Horwitz Page 9
Future Directions
Another subcortical region that is directly connected with the LMC is the thalamus,
specifically its ventral lateral, ventral posterior, medial, and centromedian groups of nuclei.
Single-unit recordings in the ventral thalamus have shown voice and speech-related neuronal
activity (Farley, 1997; McClean et al., 1990). Furthermore, the ventral thalamus has
reciprocal connections with the LMC, IFG and SMA, electrical stimulation of which also
produces vocalization in humans (Penfield and Roberts, 1959). Besides the direct role of the
ventral thalamus in vocal sensorimotor coordination of learned voice production, it
represents an important relay station of the cortico-striato-pallido-thalamo-cortical and
cortico-ponto-cerebello-thalamo-cortical loops for integration of sensorimotor information
from the basal ganglia and cerebellum. Both pathways, finally, connect the LMC with the
laryngeal motoneurons after synapsing in the reticular formation of the brainstem.
In conclusion, the central control of human voice production is organized hierarchically. As
the highest level of this control system, the human LMC is indispensible for the control of
learned but not innate vocalizations. To fulfill its function, the human LMC has direct
connections with the brainstem laryngeal motoneurons, as well as participation in a
widespread network of cortical and subcortical connections, which further indirectly connect
the LMC with laryngeal motoneurons. Thus, voluntary voice production is controlled by the
LMC and is executed through the multiple pathways, both directly and indirectly descending
to the brainstem laryngeal motoneurons. This is supported also by the fact that a combined
interruption of the direct and indirect LMC-motoneuronal pathways abolishes the ability to
produce voice and speech voluntarily, similar to bilateral lesions in the LMC alone (Bauer et
al., 1980; Urban et al., 1996).
Although over the past few years we have seen advances in understanding of the LMC's role
in human voice and speech production, there are still a number of unanswered questions
about its structural and functional organization and interactions with other brain regions in
both healthy humans and patients with neurological voice and speech disorders. In
particular, further research is needed both in humans and non-human primates for
characterization of sub-components of the LMC networks. To date, the LMC projections
have been identified using trans-synaptic tracers, such as 3H-leucine, biotin dextran amine,
and wheat germ agglutinin conjugated with horseradish peroxidase, in non-human primates
and using unconstrained probabilistic DTT in healthy humans. Both techniques provide
information about the projecting regions to and from the LMC but lack information about
whether the given connection between the LMC and the target region is direct or indirect via
other relay stations. To derive a more complete characterization of the LMC structural
networks, neuroanatomical studies using tracers that are transported in a time-dependent
manner to label synaptically-connected neuronal chains (Kelly and Strick, 2000) should be
conducted in non-human primates and further verified in healthy humans using multi-modal
neuroimaging techniques.
Future research should also focus on the organization of functional LMC networks during
different components of laryngeal behaviors and on interactions between limbic and motor
cortical parallel pathways. Similarly, given that speech production, as one of the most rapid
human motor actions, has a strong temporal dimension, the temporal characteristics of the
LMC activity remain to be explored. The significance of these studies is paramount for
future investigations of altered structural and functional links within the central voice
control system in patients with neurological voice and speech disorders.
Another unknown aspect is the laryngeal representation in the primary somatosensory cortex
and its interactions with the LMC. The earlier studies by Penfield and colleagues lacked the
mapping of the larynx representation in the primary somatosensory cortex. Since then, no
Neuroscientist. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 14.
Simonyan and Horwitz Page 10
attempts have been reported to identify the laryngeal somatosensory region, although it is
known that lesions in the inferior postcentral gyrus cause dysarthria due to the interruptions
of kinesthetic and proprioceptive feedback from the phonatory organs (Luria, 1964).
Finally, it is unknown, to date, how different neurotransmitters (e.g., dopamine, GABA)
influence and modulate the human LMC networks during voice and speech production. This
information will be crucial in identifying the target brain regions for the development of new
neuropharmacological options to modulate the LMC activity in patients with neurological
voice problems.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the Extramural and Intramural Programs of the National Institute on Deafness and
Other Communication Disorders, National Institutes of Health (R00DC009620 to K.S.).
References
Alario FX, Chainay H, Lehericy S, Cohen L. The role of the supplementary motor area (SMA) in word
production. Brain Res. 2006; 1076:129–143. [PubMed: 16480694]
Bauer G, Gerstenbrand F, Hengl W. Involuntary motor phenomena in the locked-in syndrome. J
Neurol. 1980; 223:191–198. [PubMed: 6157012]
Bernard JF, Villanueva L, Carroue J, Le Bars D. Efferent projections from the subnucleus reticularis
dorsalis (SRD): a Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin study in the rat. Neurosci Lett. 1990; 116:257–
262. [PubMed: 2173815]
Bohland JW, Guenther FH. An fMRI investigation of syllable sequence production. Neuroimage.
2006; 32:821–841. [PubMed: 16730195]
Brown S, Laird AR, Pfordresher PQ, Thelen SM, Turkeltaub P, Liotti M. The somatotopy of speech:
phonation and articulation in the human motor cortex. Brain Cogn. 2009; 70:31–41. [PubMed:
19162389]
Brown S, Ngan E, Liotti M. A larynx area in the human motor cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2008; 18:837–
845. [PubMed: 17652461]
Coude, G.; Ferrari, P.; Roda, F.; Maranesi, M.; Rozzi, S.; Fogassi, L. Ventral premotor cortex of the
macaque monkey controls conditioned vocalization. Society for Neuroscience; Chicago, IL: 2009.
de Zubicaray GI, McMahon KL. Auditory context effects in picture naming investigated with event-
related fMRI. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2009; 9:260–269. [PubMed: 19679762]
Dujardin E, Jurgens U. Afferents of vocalization-controlling periaqueductal regions in the squirrel
monkey. Brain Res. 2005; 1034:114–131. [PubMed: 15713263]
Farley GR. Neural firing in ventrolateral thalamic nucleus during conditioned vocal behavior in cats.
Exp Brain Res. 1997; 115:493–506. [PubMed: 9262204]
Fiebach CJ, Friederici AD, Smith EE, Swinney D. Lateral inferotemporal cortex maintains conceptual-
semantic representations in verbal working memory. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007; 19:2035–2049.
[PubMed: 17892385]
Foester, O. Motorische Felder und Bahnen. Springer; Berlin: 1936.
Galgano J, Froud K. Evidence of the voice-related cortical potential: an electroencephalographic study.
Neuroimage. 2008; 41:1313–1323. [PubMed: 18495493]
Ghosh SS, Tourville JA, Guenther FH. A neuroimaging study of premotor lateralization and cerebellar
involvement in the production of phonemes and syllables. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;
51:1183–1202. [PubMed: 18664692]
Greenlee JD, Oya H, Kawasaki H, Volkov IO, Kaufman OP, Kovach C, Howard MA, Brugge JF. A
functional connection between inferior frontal gyrus and orofacial motor cortex in human. J
Neurophysiol. 2004; 92:1153–1164. [PubMed: 15056683]
Guenther FH. Cortical interactions underlying the production of speech sounds. J Commun Disord.
2006; 39:350–365. [PubMed: 16887139]
Neuroscientist. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 14.
Simonyan and Horwitz Page 11
Hage SR, Jurgens U. On the role of the pontine brainstem in vocal pattern generation: a telemetric
single-unit recording study in the squirrel monkey. J Neurosci. 2006; 26:7105–7115. [PubMed:
16807339]
Hannig S, Jurgens U. Projections of the ventrolateral pontine vocalization area in the squirrel monkey.
Exp Brain Res. 2006; 169:92–105. [PubMed: 16292643]
Hast MH, Fischer JM, Wetzel AB, Thompson VE. Cortical motor representation of the laryngeal
muscles in Macaca mulatta. Brain Res. 1974; 73:229–240. [PubMed: 4208647]
Hocking J, McMahon KL, de Zubicaray GI. Semantic context and visual feature effects in object
naming: an fMRI study using arterial spin labeling. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009; 21:1571–1583.
[PubMed: 18823254]
Horwitz B, Amunts K, Bhattacharyya R, Patkin D, Jeffries K, Zilles K, Braun AR. Activation of
Broca's area during the production of spoken and signed language: a combined cytoarchitectonic
mapping and PET analysis. Neuropsychologia. 2003; 41:1868–1876. [PubMed: 14572520]
Iwatsubo T, Kuzuhara S, Kanemitsu A, Shimada H, Toyokura Y. Corticofugal projections to the motor
nuclei of the brainstem and spinal cord in humans. Neurology. 1990; 40:309–312. [PubMed:
2300253]
Jurgens U. On the elicitability of vocalization from the cortical larynx area. Brain Res. 1974; 81:564–
566. [PubMed: 4215545]
Jurgens U. Projections from the cortical larynx area in the squirrel monkey. Exp Brain Res. 1976;
25:401–411. [PubMed: 821768]
Jurgens U. Neural pathways underlying vocal control. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2002; 26:235–258.
[PubMed: 11856561]
Jurgens U, Ehrenreich L. The descending motorcortical pathway to the laryngeal motoneurons in the
squirrel monkey. Brain Res. 2007; 1148:90–95. [PubMed: 17362883]
Kelly RM, Strick PL. Rabies as a transneuronal tracer of circuits in the central nervous system. J
Neurosci Methods. 2000; 103:63–71. [PubMed: 11074096]
Khedr EM, Aref EE. Electrophysiological study of vocal-fold mobility disorders using a magnetic
stimulator. Eur J Neurol. 2002; 9:259–267. [PubMed: 11985634]
Kirzinger A, Jurgens U. Vocalization-correlated single-unit activity in the brain stem of the squirrel
monkey. Exp Brain Res. 1991; 84:545–560. [PubMed: 1864326]
Kunzle H. Bilateral projections from precentral motor cortex to the putamen and other parts of the
basal ganglia. An autoradiographic study in Macaca fascicularis. Brain Res. 1975; 88:195–209.
[PubMed: 50112]
Kuypers HG. Corticobular connexions to the pons and lower brain-stem in man: an anatomical study.
Brain. 1958; 81:364–388. [PubMed: 13596471]
Loucks TM, Poletto CJ, Simonyan K, Reynolds CL, Ludlow CL. Human brain activation during
phonation and exhalation: common volitional control for two upper airway functions. Neuroimage.
2007; 36:131–143. [PubMed: 17428683]
Luria, AR. Factors and forms of aphasia.. In: de Reuck, AVS.; O'Connor, M., editors. Disorders of
language. Churchill; London: 1964. p. 143-167.
Luthe L, Hausler U, Jurgens U. Neuronal activity in the medulla oblongata during vocalization. A
single-unit recording study in the squirrel monkey. Behav Brain Res. 2000; 116:197–210.
[PubMed: 11080551]
Matsuzaka Y, Aizawa H, Tanji J. A motor area rostral to the supplementary motor area
(presupplementary motor area) in the monkey: neuronal activity during a learned motor task. J
Neurophysiol. 1992; 68:653–662. [PubMed: 1432040]
McClean MD, Dostrovsky JO, Lee L, Tasker RR. Somatosensory neurons in human thalamus respond
to speech-induced orofacial movements. Brain Res. 1990; 513:343–347. [PubMed: 2350707]
Milojevic B, Hast MH. Cortical Motor Centers of the Laryngeal Muscles in the Cat and Dog. Ann Otol
Rhinol Laryngol. 1964; 73:979–988. [PubMed: 14239542]
Monnier M, Willi H. [The integrative activity of the nervous system of a mesorhombencephalic
anencephalus. II. Anatomical part.]. Monatsschr Psychiatr Neurol. 1953; 126:259–273. [PubMed:
13154256]
Neuroscientist. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 14.
Simonyan and Horwitz Page 12
Olthoff A, Baudewig J, Kruse E, Dechent P. Cortical sensorimotor control in vocalization: a functional
magnetic resonance imaging study. Laryngoscope. 2008; 118:2091–2096. [PubMed: 18758379]
Ozdemir E, Norton A, Schlaug G. Shared and distinct neural correlates of singing and speaking.
Neuroimage. 2006; 33:628–635. [PubMed: 16956772]
Papoutsi M, de Zwart JA, Jansma JM, Pickering MJ, Bednar JA, Horwitz B. From phonemes to
articulatory codes: an fMRI study of the role of Broca's area in speech production. Cereb Cortex.
2009; 19:2156–2165. [PubMed: 19181696]
Peeva MG, Guenther FH, Tourville JA, Nieto-Castanon A, Anton JL, Nazarian B, Alario FX. Distinct
representations of phonemes, syllables, and supra-syllabic sequences in the speech production
network. Neuroimage. 50:626–638. [PubMed: 20035884]
Penfield W, Bordley E. Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cortex as studied by electrical
stimulation. Brain. 1937; 60:389–443.
Penfield, W.; Roberts, L. Speech and brain mechanisms. Princeton University Press; Princeton: 1959.
Penfield W, Welch K. The supplementary motor area of the cerebral cortex; a clinical and
experimental study. AMA Arch Neurol Psychiatry. 1951; 66:289–317.
Peschke C, Ziegler W, Kappes J, Baumgaertner A. Auditory-motor integration during fast repetition:
the neuronal correlates of shadowing. Neuroimage. 2009; 47:392–402. [PubMed: 19345269]
Price, JL. Vocalization and the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex.. In: Davis, PJ.; Fletcher, NH.,
editors. Vocal Fold Physiology: Controlling Complexity and Chaos. Singular Publishing Group;
San Diego: 1996. p. 171-185.
Ramsay SC, Adams L, Murphy K, Corfield DR, Grootoonk S, Bailey DL, Frackowiak RS, Guz A.
Regional cerebral blood flow during volitional expiration in man: a comparison with volitional
inspiration. J Physiol. 1993; 461:85–101. [PubMed: 8350282]
Riecker A, Ackermann H, Wildgruber D, Dogil G, Grodd W. Opposite hemispheric lateralization
effects during speaking and singing at motor cortex, insula and cerebellum. Neuroreport. 2000;
11:1997–2000. [PubMed: 10884059]
Riecker A, Brendel B, Ziegler W, Erb M, Ackermann H. The influence of syllable onset complexity
and syllable frequency on speech motor control. Brain Lang. 2008; 107:102–113. [PubMed:
18294683]
Rodel RM, Olthoff A, Tergau F, Simonyan K, Kraemer D, Markus H, Kruse E. Human cortical motor
representation of the larynx as assessed by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS).
Laryngoscope. 2004; 114:918–922. [PubMed: 15126757]
Scheiner E, Hammerschmidt K, Jurgens U, Zwirner P. The influence of hearing impairment on
preverbal emotional vocalizations of infants. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2004; 56:27–40. [PubMed:
14767158]
Shiba K, Umezaki T, Zheng Y, Miller AD. The nucleus retroambigualis controls laryngeal muscle
activity during vocalization in the cat. Exp Brain Res. 1997; 115:513–519. [PubMed: 9262206]
Shima K, Tanji J. Both supplementary and presupplementary motor areas are crucial for the temporal
organization of multiple movements. J Neurophysiol. 1998; 80:3247–3260. [PubMed: 9862919]
Simonyan K, Jurgens U. Cortico-cortical projections of the motorcortical larynx area in the rhesus
monkey. Brain Res. 2002; 949:23–31. [PubMed: 12213296]
Simonyan K, Jurgens U. Efferent subcortical projections of the laryngeal motorcortex in the rhesus
monkey. Brain Res. 2003; 974:43–59. [PubMed: 12742623]
Simonyan K, Jurgens U. Afferent cortical connections of the motor cortical larynx area in the rhesus
monkey. Neuroscience. 2005a; 130:133–149. [PubMed: 15561431]
Simonyan K, Jurgens U. Afferent subcortical connections into the motor cortical larynx area in the
rhesus monkey. Neuroscience. 2005b; 130:119–131. [PubMed: 15561430]
Simonyan K, Ostuni J, Ludlow CL, Horwitz B. Functional but not structural networks of the human
laryngeal motor cortex show left hemispheric lateralization during syllable but not breathing
production. J Neurosci. 2009; 29:14912–14923. [PubMed: 19940187]
Simonyan K, Saad ZS, Loucks TM, Poletto CJ, Ludlow CL. Functional neuroanatomy of human
voluntary cough and sniff production. Neuroimage. 2007; 37:401–409. [PubMed: 17574873]
Neuroscientist. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 14.
Simonyan and Horwitz Page 13
Tanji J, Shima K. Supplementary motor cortex in organization of movement. Eur Neurol 36 Suppl.
1996; 1:13–19.
Terumitsu M, Fujii Y, Suzuki K, Kwee IL, Nakada T. Human primary motor cortex shows
hemispheric specialization for speech. Neuroreport. 2006; 17:1091–1095. [PubMed: 16837833]
Tettamanti M, Rotondi I, Perani D, Scotti G, Fazio F, Cappa SF, Moro A. Syntax without language:
neurobiological evidence for cross-domain syntactic computations. Cortex. 2009; 45:825–838.
[PubMed: 19111290]
Thoms G, Jurgens U. Common input of the cranial motor nuclei involved in phonation in squirrel
monkey. Exp Neurol. 1987; 95:85–99. [PubMed: 3792484]
Tyler LK, Shafto MA, Randall B, Wright P, Marslen-Wilson WD, Stamatakis EA. Preserving
syntactic processing across the adult life span: the modulation of the frontotemporal language
system in the context of age-related atrophy. Cereb Cortex. 2010; 20:352–364. [PubMed:
19505991]
Urban PP, Hopf HC, Connemann B, Hundemer HP, Koehler J. The course of cortico-hypoglossal
projections in the human brainstem. Functional testing using transcranial magnetic stimulation.
Brain. 1996; 119(Pt 3):1031–1038. [PubMed: 8673480]
van de Ven V, Esposito F, Christoffels IK. Neural network of speech monitoring overlaps with overt
speech production and comprehension networks: a sequential spatial and temporal ICA study.
Neuroimage. 2009; 47:1982–1991. [PubMed: 19481159]
VanderHorst VG, Terasawa E, Ralston HJ 3rd. Monosynaptic projections from the nucleus
retroambiguus region to laryngeal motoneurons in the rhesus monkey. Neuroscience. 2001;
107:117–125. [PubMed: 11744252]
Willems RM, Ozyurek A, Hagoort P. Differential roles for left inferior frontal and superior temporal
cortex in multimodal integration of action and language. Neuroimage. 2009; 47:1992–2004.
[PubMed: 19497376]
Wilson SM, Saygin AP, Sereno MI, Iacoboni M. Listening to speech activates motor areas involved in
speech production. Nat Neurosci. 2004; 7:701–702. [PubMed: 15184903]
Wise RJ, Greene J, Buchel C, Scott SK. Brain regions involved in articulation. Lancet. 1999;
353:1057–1061. [PubMed: 10199354]
Woolsey CN, Settlage PH, Meyer DR, Sencer W, Pinto Hamuy T, Travis AM. Patterns of localization
in precentral and “supplementary” motor areas and their relation to the concept of a premotor area.
Res Publ Assoc Res Nerv Ment Dis. 1952; 30:238–264. [PubMed: 12983675]
Zheng ZZ, Munhall KG, Johnsrude IS. Functional overlap between regions involved in speech
perception and in monitoring one's own voice during speech production. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;
22:1770–1781. [PubMed: 19642886]
Neuroscientist. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 14.
Simonyan and Horwitz Page 14
Figure 1. (A) Schematic view of the vocal and respiratory tracts
Voice originates in the larynx. First, the expiratory airflow from the lungs reaches the larynx
through the trachea, where it sets the closed vocal fold tissue into self-excited oscillatations,
due to which the larynx becomes the source of voice sound. Further, pressure from the vocal
fold oscillations is resonated through the vocal tract and radiated from the mouth as voice.
(B) Schematic sequence of events preceding voice production. (1). The vocal folds close
immideatly prior to voice production; (2) subglottal air pressure builds up below the vocal
folds during exhalation; (3) lower and upper edge of the vocal folds separate subsequently
with the release of air and sound generation; (4) the vocal folds re-approximate, starting
from their lower edge, and (5) the vocal folds close completely before the next sound
production. (C) Superior and lateral views of the human larynx. Intrinsic laryngeal muscles
and cartilages. TA – thyroarytenoid muscle; LCA – lateral cricoarytenoid muscle; PCA –
posterior cricoarytenoid muscle; IA – interarytenoid muscle; CT - cricothyroid muscle. The
arrows show the directions of the muscle contractions. (D) Schematic presentation of the
laryngeal muscle function. The left column shows the location of the cartilages and the edge
of the vocal folds when each of the laryngeal muscles is active. The arrows indicate the
directions of the force exerted. 1. thyroid cartilage; 2. cricoid cartilage; 3. arytenoid
cartilages; 4. vocal ligament; 5. posterior cricoarytenoid ligament. The middle column
shows the laryngeal view. The right column shows contours of the frontal section at the
middle of the membranous portion of the vocal fold. The dotted line shows a state in which
no muscle is activated (reprinted from Hirano, 1981 with permission of Springer Science +
Business Media).
Neuroscientist. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 14.
Simonyan and Horwitz Page 15
Figure 2. Hierarchical organization of central voice control in humans and non-human primates
The figure depicts different levels of the voice control system. The lowest level (Subsystem
I) is represented by the brainstem and spinal cord sensorimotor phonatory nuclei. This
subsystem is responsible for the coordination of laryngeal, articulatory and respiratory
control during production of innate vocalizations. The higher level within this system
(Subsystem II) is represented by the PAG, ACC and limbic input structures, such as the
hypothalamus, midline thalamus, amygdala, red nucleus, preoptic region, septum. This
subsystem is responsible for initiation of vocalizations and control of voluntary emotional
vocalizations. The highest level is represented by the laryngeal/orofacial motor cortex with
its input and output regions (Subsystem III). This subsystem is responsible for voluntary
vocal motor control of speech and song production. The dotted lines show simplified
connections between different regions within the voice controlling system.
Neuroscientist. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 14.
Simonyan and Horwitz Page 16
Figure 3. Laryngeal motor cortical representation
Schematic views of body representation within the motor cortex (A) in humans (“motor
homunculus” according to (Penfield and Bordley, 1937)) and (B) in the rhesus monkey
(“motor simiculus” according to (Woolsey et al., 1952), reprinted from Fadiga et al., 2000
with permission from Elsevier). (C) The laryngeal motor cortical region in humans as
defined in neuroimaging studies. The colored circles represent the reported peaks of
activation in the following studies of syllable production: orange - (Bohland and Guenther,
2006); purple - (Olthoff et al., 2008); light blue - (Terumitsu et al., 2006); green - (Loucks et
al., 2007); blue - (Brown et al., 2008); red -(Wilson et al., 2004); black - (Simonyan et al.,
2009); white - (Riecker et al., 2008), and yellow - (Peeva et al.). CS - central sulcus. (D) The
laryngeal motor cortical region in the rhesus monkey. Topographical representation of the
laryngeal muscles: cricothyroid - right-angled triangle; thyroarytenoid - circle; combination
of the circothyroid and thyroarytenoid - encircled right-angled triangle, and extrinsic
laryngeal muscles - square. sca - sulcus subcentralis anterior (subcentral dimple) (reprinted
from (Hast et al., 1974) with permission from Elsevier).
Neuroscientist. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 14.
Simonyan and Horwitz Page 17
Figure 4. Cortical and subcortical networks of the laryngeal motor cortex
Block diagrams illustrate the reciprocal (pink box), outgoing (green box) and incoming
(purple box) connections of the laryngeal motor cortex as defined using neuroanatomical
tracing studies in non-human primates and diffusion tensor tractography in humans. Asterisk
(*) indicates that projection from the laryngeal motor cortex to the nucleus ambiguus exists
only in humans but not in non-human primates.
Neuroscientist. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 April 14.
मंगलवार, 25 अगस्त 2015
सदस्यता लें
टिप्पणियाँ भेजें (Atom)
कोई टिप्पणी नहीं:
एक टिप्पणी भेजें